- Scott H Young - https://www.scotthyoung.com/blog -

How Much Transfer Should We Expect Between Skills?

Transfer occurs when learning one thing helps you learn or do something else. It’s arguably the most important issue in educational psychology. After all, we spend years in the classroom. One would hope the things we learn make us more productive on the job, conscientious as citizens or intelligent in our daily lives.

Yet it turns out the titular question is really difficult to answer. This is because the answer depends on three different questions, each of which is itself incredibly complicated:

  1. How do we perform skills?
  2. How do we acquire them?
  3. What sorts of skills are used in real life?

About two years ago, I started a research project to learn everything I could about transfer. It quickly became clear that understanding transfer depended on answering the other three questions, so my research broadened drastically.

Despite two years of research and a lot of writing about transfer (including a chapter of my book [1]), I still don’t have a definitive answer for how much transfer we can expect. But I’d like to share at least how I’ve come to think about the issue.

Question One: How Do We Perform Skills?

Listen to this article

Essentially, transfer is a product of overlap. We should expect transfer from one skill to another when something in those two skills is essentially the same.

Nearly every well-articulated theory of transfer is an overlap theory. What differs between theories is the “something” which potentially overlaps. Let’s consider some candidates:

Only the first two seem to be demonstrably false. The mind is not like a muscle [8], nor is it a simplistic stimulus-response device.

I find the information-processing perspective of cognitive science to be the most persuasive. The mind is essentially computational (although quite different from computers made out of transistors). Answering the question of transfer depends on how the algorithms for skills are composed.

From this perspective, what seems safe to say is that skills can only reliably influence each other if they overlap in either their procedures or knowledge. If two skills don’t use the same process, and the knowledge they depend on is different, there shouldn’t be much (if any) transfer.1 [9]

Question Two: How Do We Acquire Skills?

How the mind represents skills is only one part of understanding transfer. There’s also the issue of how we acquire these representations.

Here, a few things seem pertinent:

On top of these psychological details, there’s the issue of pedagogy. There are often multiple methods for teaching something. Those methods have varying levels of generality and can apply to a broader or narrower range of concepts. Transfer, then, also depends on how something is taught.

My sense from reading this research is that there are a few pitfalls to avoid:

Question Three: How are Skills Used in Real Life?

The final question is about the kinds of cognitive activities people engage in during work, school and daily life.

While possibly the most relevant question of the three, it is also the hardest to answer. Few studies have looked systematically at the kinds of cognitive skills people regularly employ at work and at home.

Critics allege that much of what we learn in school has little real-world application.

John Anderson reports [17] that employers either want very basic skills (which the educational system should already be teaching) or highly specific skills that no universal curriculum could aspire to. Similarly, Peter Cappelli argues [18] that surveys show employers tend not to highly value academic skills.

Jean Lave found [19] that the mathematics taught in school is rarely what people use in their daily routines.

Bryan Caplan argues [20] that much of education is signaling—showing others we have smarts, conformity and work-ethic—rather than cultivating those abilities. From this perspective, the real purpose of schools is to sort people, not to teach useful skills. School knowledge doesn’t apply to real life, Caplan argues, because that’s not the function it serves in society.

Supporters of education argue that just because most skills aren’t used doesn’t mean they’re useless. We may become specialists of one sort or another, but unless we assign everyone a career at birth, we want to maintain maximum flexibility until our adult years. A side-effect of this desire to maintain flexibility is that we end up learning a lot that we don’t ever end up using, or that is insufficiently concrete to apply in practice without further training.

Similarly, education proponents point out [21] that just because a skill isn’t used doesn’t mean it can’t be. Statistics is a powerful tool for reasoning about many important issues. The fact that people are statistically naive shows we should be teaching them more, not declaring it impotent!

Whether we apply a skill in a new context depends on the effort it requires. Practice can lower the effort of applying a skill in real life and create more situations where it is convenient to use. Imagine rewinding time to before widespread literacy; it would be perverse to argue that reading isn’t useful because most people don’t read.

My perspective is that, while many academic subjects aren’t used, it’s difficult to appreciate a subject until you learn it. Given that we generally cannot “infer” a domain of knowledge simply by encountering problems that apply it, this argument favors accumulating more knowledge than you think you’ll need.

Side Note: Transfer of Effort, Motivation and “Noncognitive” Skills

Thus far I’ve been looking at transfer through a cognitive lens. A somewhat separate issue is the transfer of things like the ability to focus and study for long periods of time, the ability to resist temptations, or the motivation to want to learn.

Many of the same arguments used above apply to noncognitive skills as well. There’s unlikely to be something like a general faculty you can improve with bulk training efforts for motivation, persistence or focus.

That said, the arguments about transfer here are somewhat different because these are not complex skills (in the information processing sense of “complex”). Sitting at your desk and focusing for eight hours may be difficult, but it’s not computationally demanding.

Thus while I think it’s ridiculous to imagine improving your memory by memorizing Latin verbs, it’s not ridiculous to imagine improving your ability to focus by regularly sitting down to focus. Believing that cognitive skills are highly specific, and should show limited transfer, is not logically incompatible with a more general transfer of effort or persistence.

I might write a fuller summary of my views later, but I want to clarify that while “speaking French” and “being able to focus” are both discussed as skills, they have a different locus of difficulty. French is hard because there are lots of words, grammar and mental processes you need to perform it. Focusing is hard because the naive reward center of your brain wants to go on Twitter instead.

Summarizing My Thoughts on Transfer

If you take one thing from this article, it should be that transfer is complicated.

As a practical takeaway, I would agree with roughly the following:

Footnotes

  1. I’m leaving aside analogies. Analogies suggest two domains that have nothing to do with each other may actually be a source of transfer. But another way of saying this is that analogies suggest an abstract commonality between two superficially different domains. When analogies are useful, they tend to share an overlapping deep structure, so I would include them as an “overlap” theory, even if the overlap is an active process rather than the nature of the mental representations themselves.
  2. The science on reading [25] seems relevant here: careful experimentation seems to indicate that fluent readers decode words letter-by-letter, even though we may feel like we recognize meaning holistically as we look at text.
  3. For instance, I tend to think learning Chinese will help with later learning Spanish, even though there is virtually no overlap in the vocabulary or syntax. Instead, what is transferred is a more abstract understanding of languages and how they are learned. [26]
  4. This is a major misconception I had when I wrote my “do the real thing [27]” piece.